The hardware and bandwidth for this mirror is donated by dogado GmbH, the Webhosting and Full Service-Cloud Provider. Check out our Wordpress Tutorial.
If you wish to report a bug, or if you are interested in having us mirror your free-software or open-source project, please feel free to contact us at mirror[@]dogado.de.

Cumulative History

Alexander (Sasha) Pastukhov

2023-09-13

Certain stimuli, such as a Necker cube depicted below, are compatible with several, typically two, comparably likely perceptual interpretations. In case of the Necker cube, one can perceive as “upwards” or “downwards” and during continuous viewing the perception alternates between these alternatives (these alternations are schematically depicted on the right).

A distribution of these so-called dominance phases typically has a right-skewed distribution that is frequently fitted using Gamma distribution.

However, the individual dominance phases show a subtle but consistent serial dependence, see, for example, (van Ee 2009). This serial dependence is thought to reflect accumulation of slow adaptation (Pastukhov et al. 2013) or prediction error (Weilnhammer et al. 2017) for the dominant percept. This slow accumulation process can be described via a homogeneous first order process. A brief summary on the formula and examples of accumulation over time for different initial values, signal strength, and time constants are presented below. For further details, please refer to (Pastukhov and Braun 2011).


The cumulative history for a perceptual state is computed via a homogeneous first order process (for details on first order linear differential equations please refer to chapter 2 in Wilson (1999)): \[\tag{1}\frac{dh_i}{dt} = \frac{1}{\tau} (-h_i + S_i(t))\]

where \(\tau\) is the time constant, \(h_i\) is cumulative history and \(S_i(t)\) is current signal level for for the ith perceptual state, so that \[\tag{2} S(t) = \begin{cases} 1 & \text{if state $i$ is dominant}\\ 0 & \text{if state $i$ is suppressed}\\ S_{mixed} & \text{if it is a mixed/transition phase and $0 ≥ S_{mixed} ≥1 $} \end{cases}\]

where \(S_{mixed}\) corresponds to the mixed_state parameter that can be either specified (the fit_cumhist() function uses a default of 0.5) or fitted. The general solution for the equation (see Theorem 1 in Wilson (1999), chapter 2, page 15) is

\[\tag{3}h_i(t) = A e^{-t/\tau} + \frac{1}{\tau} \int_{0}^{t} e^{-(t'-t)/\tau} S(t') dt'\]

where \(A\) is chosen to satisfy the initial condition. Assuming a constant signal \(S\), we obtain \[\tag{4}h_i(t) = A e^{-t/\tau} + S_i \cdot (1 - e^{-t/\tau})\]

For the cumulative history, we are interested in \(h_i(t + \Delta t)\): a change following a dominance phase that starts at time \(t\), ends at time \(t + \Delta t\), and has a constant signal strength \(S_i\). Assuming that a dominance phase starts at \(t=0\) and substituting into the equation 4, \(h_i(0) = A\). In other words, constant \(A\) is equal to cumulative history state before the dominance phase onset and, therefore, the relative signal strength during the dominance phase is determined by the difference between the signal strength and the initial cumulative history value: \(S_i - h_i(0)\). Thus \[\tag{5} h_i(\Delta t) = h_i(0) + (S - h_i(0)) \cdot (1 - e^{-\Delta t/\tau})\] \[\tag{6} h_i(\Delta t) = S + (h_i(0) - S) \cdot e^{-\Delta t/\tau}\]

The figure below shows accumulation over time for three different initial values (\(x(0)\)), signal strength (\(S\)), and and time constants (\(tau\)). Note that the package allows to either specify and fit both the time constant (argument tau in fit_cumhist() function) and the initial history value at the block (history_init argument).

As for a bistable case there are two history states (one for each perceptual state), we compute a history as a difference of cumulative histories \[\tag{7}\Delta h(t, \tau) = h_{suppressed}(t, \tau) - h_{dominant}(t, \tau) \] where \(h_{dominant}\) and \(h_{suppressed}\) are history states for the currently dominant and suppressed states, respectively. E.g., if a left eye dominates during following phase, \(h_{dominant} = h_{left}\) and \(h_{suppressed} = h_{right}\) and vice versa.

References

Pastukhov, Alexander, and Jochen Braun. 2011. Cumulative history quantifies the role of neural adaptation in multistable perception.” Journal of Vision 11 (10): 12–12. https://doi.org/10.1167/11.10.12.
Pastukhov, Alexander, Pedro E García-Rodríguez, Joachim Haenicke, Antoni Guillamon, Gustavo Deco, and Jochen Braun. 2013. Multi-stable perception balances stability and sensitivity.” Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience 7 (17): 17. https://doi.org/10.3389/fncom.2013.00017.
van Ee, Raymond. 2009. Stochastic variations in sensory awareness are driven by noisy neuronal adaptation: evidence from serial correlations in perceptual bistability.” Journal of the Optical Society of America A 26 (12): 2612. https://doi.org/10.1364/JOSAA.26.002612.
Weilnhammer, Veith A., Heiner Stuke, Guido Hesselmann, Philipp Sterzer, and Katharina Schmack. 2017. A predictive coding account of bistable perception - a model-based fMRI study.” Edited by Jean Daunizeau. PLOS Computational Biology 13 (5): e1005536. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005536.
Wilson, Hugh R. 1999. Spikes, Decisions & Actions: Dynamical Foundations of Neuroscience. Oxford UK: Oxford University Press.

These binaries (installable software) and packages are in development.
They may not be fully stable and should be used with caution. We make no claims about them.
Health stats visible at Monitor.