The hardware and bandwidth for this mirror is donated by dogado GmbH, the Webhosting and Full Service-Cloud Provider. Check out our Wordpress Tutorial.
If you wish to report a bug, or if you are interested in having us mirror your free-software or open-source project, please feel free to contact us at mirror[@]dogado.de.

Using justifier in study design

Gjalt-Jorn Peters

2023-03-05

Why justify?

Planning a scientific study in psychology entails taking very many decisions. Some of these decisions are taken without thinking about it: for example, given the default to use either Type II (SPSS) or Type III (R) Sums of Squares in Analysis of Variance analyses, users of either package will sometimes follow that default without realising this constitutes an implicit decision. However, ideally, researchers make these decisions deliberately, and ideally, they have justifications for these decisions.

In fact, a recent blog post called upon researchers in psychology to #JustifyEverything:

My new motto is ‘Justify Everything’ (it also works as a hashtag: #JustifyEverything).

Daniel Lakens, 2019

And indeed, if researchers would justify all their decisions, this has the potential of yielding a wealth of benefits. First, researchers may become more aware of the decisions they take, enabling them to take those decisons more deliberately. This would decrease the much-lamented unquestioning reliance on defaults in the used software. Second, review of study proposals has the potential to become much more efficient, if the decisions that were taken and their justifications can be extracted and parsed automatically. Third, once justifing everything becomes common practice, analysing these decisions and their justifications can be very insightful, for example showing where training of researchers may be most beneficial (i.e. areas where decisions and their justifications are based on outdated assumptions). Fourth, documenting decisions and justifications means that a lot of aspects of a study’s methods and analyses are necessarily documented in a machine-readable way. This means that when conducting evidence syntheses, the information can be automatically extracted, and no longer needs to be coded from the manuscript.

However, for this potential to be realised, justifications need to be documented in a uniform format. Ideally, this is a format that is simultaneously userfriendly enough to be used by humans as they document their meeting minutes or notes, and structured enough to be machine-readable so as to enable automatic extraction and parsing. The justifier package provides this format. The basic introduction of justifier is in the general introduction vignette, and this vignette presents a simple example using a justification framework for planning psychological studies.1

Study design decisions

In this example framework, we will limit ourselves to four decisions that a researcher always takes for any study they undertake. The first is to establish the research question or questions, or if appropriate, hypothesis or hypothesis. The second is to determine which method wil be used. The third is deciding how many data points need to be sampled (for example, how many participants will be recruited). The fourth is whether to preregister the study.2

The example study is an applied study into the social-cognitive determinants of getting one’s ecstasy pills tested. In the Netherlands, there are drug test labs where one can bring drugs to anonymously get those tested and find out the ingredients and their dose. Ectasy pills ideally contain only one active ingredient: 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamin (MDMA). However, pills can contains varying doses of MDMA (in 2017, the mean MDMA dose was 187mg of MDMA; 1% of the pills had a dose under 35mg of MDMA and 70% over 140mg of MDMA). In addition, pills can contain contaminants (in 2017, 87.6% of the pills tested in the Netherlands contained only MDMA as active ingrdient).

This example is chosen for two reasons. First, it’s my topic, so this way I don’t have to learn about another domain to write it. Second, it’s the same example as the one used in the intervention development vignette, so that readers won’t have to familiarize themselves with two scenarios.

Research question

The first decision is in a way the simplest one: what exactly does one want to study? However, this research question isn’t arbitrary. In this world of finite, often scarce, resources, justification of why to do a study in the first place is perhaps one of the most important considerations.

In this applied example study, the research question is "What are the most important determinants of getting one's ecstasy tested?". The answer to this research question is required to develop and effective interventions to promote ecstasy pill testing. It is important to promote ecstasy pill testing because if ecstasy users do not get their pills tested, they are exposed to two risks. First, they may unknowingly ingest contaminants; and second, proper dosing (~ 1-1.5 mg of MDMA per kg of body weight) becomes hard if the dose in a pill is unknown. To minimize the likelihood of incidents and accidental intoxication, testing is a required step.

Method

Often, the appropriate method follows from a combination of the type of research question and knowledge about the relevant domain. Some research questions require qualitative research; some quantitative research; and some are best answered by research synthesis. However, within each of these, many choices have to be made. To name a few examples, for qualitative research, which analysis approach is most appropriate and how to sample participants; for quantitative research, whether to use an experimental design or not and which operationalisations to use; and for a research syntesis, which bibliographic databases to use and whether to extract effect size esimates.

In the example study, there are two kinds of considerations. On the one hand, logistical considerations (available time and money. On the other hand, available prior knowledge from earlier research. On the one hand, one needs to know enough about potential determinants and sub-determinants to develop a sufficiently comprehensive questionnaire. On the other hand, one can keep doing qualitative studies and learning a little bit more ad infinitum, while in reality, one often has to work with (severely) limited funds and time.

Sample size

Once the method is clear, for empirical research, it becomes possible to plan the required sample size. For example, in this example study, we aim to map the importance of a series of behavioral determinants to then inform intervention development. That means we desire sufficiently accurate estimates of determinants’ relevance. To obtain a confidence interval with a half-width of around .1, 400 participants suffice (Moinester & Gottfried, 2014).

Preregistration

Preregistation is an excellent example of a decision that is taken for every single study, yet often without the researchers’ awareness. As with any decision taken during study planning, there are no absolute ‘right’ or ‘wrong’ answers: the justification of the decision is often more important than the outcome.

Example study decisions in justifier format

In this section, the decisions explained above are shows in the justifier format.

Framework specification

In one of the ‘justified markdown’ files, for example meeting minutes, researchers would specify the framework to use. In this case, this specification would look like this:

---
justifier:
  -
    scope: global   # Can also be local or universal
    framework: justifier-example-study-framework-specification.jmd
  -
    scope: local
    date: 2019-03-06
---

This justifier fragment contains two bits of metadata, the first applying to all files in this directory (scope: global) and the second only to this file (scope: local; scope: universal could have been used to apply the metadata to all files read by justifier in a given project). The first fragment specifies the framework to apply (justifier-example-study-framework-specification.jmd). This framework contains information about contexts of decisions and conditions that can apply to decisions, justifications, assertions, and sources.

Research question

---
decision:

  ### Type and decision can be the same, but it is also possible that there
  ### are multiple decisions of the same type, for example relating to different
  ### aspects of sample size planning or selection of operationalisations.
  type: research_question
  id: research_question

  ### The value is the decision that is taken (the chosen alternative). For decisions
  ### with a type, this will be checked against the allowed values specified in the
  ### justifier framework, if such a framework is available.
  value: "What are the most important determinants of getting one's ecstasy tested?"

  ### The label and description are human-readable free-text fields.
  label: "The answer to this research question is required to develop and effective interventions to promote ecstasy pill testing."
  description: "To minimize the likelihood of incidents and accidental intoxication, testing is a required step."

  justification:
    -
      id: without_testing_users_may_ingest_contaminants
      label: "Some XTC pills are contaminated, and one needs to test them to be aware of the pill contents."
      assertion:
        -
          id: some_pills_are_contaminated
          source: vogels_2009
          label: "Not all produced XTC pills contain only MDMA as active ingredient."
          evidence_type: "empirical study"
    -
      id: proper_dosing_requires_knowing_dose
      label: "Proper dosing (~ 1-1.5 mg of MDMA per kg of body weight) becomes hard if the dose in a pill is unknown."
      assertion:
        id: dosing_requires_pill_content
        label: "Determining the dose of MDMA one uses requires knowing both one's body weight and the pill dose."
        source:
          id: brunt_2012
          label: "Brunt, Koeter, Niesink & van den Brink (2012)"
          evidence_type: "empirical study"
---

Method

---
decision:

  ### Type and decision can be the same, but it is also possible that there
  ### are multiple decisions of the same type, for example relating to different
  ### aspects of sample size planning or selection of operationalisations.
  type: global_study_method
  id: global_study_method

  ### The value is the decision that is taken (the chosen alternative). For decisions
  ### with a type, this will be checked against the allowed values specified in the
  ### justifier framework, if such a framework is available.
  value: quantitative

  ### The label and description are human-readable free-text fields.
  label: "We will conduct a quantitative study."
  description: "Here, a decision can be explained more in detail (e.g. describing how the justifications relate to each other)."

  justification:
    -
      id: enough_known_about_testing_determinants
      label: "We have enough information available to develop a questionnaire that is likely to measure the most important determinants and sub-determinants of XTC testing."
      assertion:
        -
          id: testing_xtc_is_reasoned
          source: phd_peters_2008
          label: "Previous research indicates that getting one's ecstasy tested (or not) is largely a reasoned behavior"
        -
          id: there_is_qualitative_research_about_testing
          source: phd_peters_2008
          label: "There exists qualitative research about why people get their ecstasy tested"
    -
      id: limited_time
      label: "We have to finish this study before the end of 2019."
      assertion:
        -
          id: project_deadline
          label: "The deadline for this project is december 2019."
          source:
            id: project_proposal
            label: "The original proposal for this project as funded by our funder, see the document."
            year: 2017
---

Sample size

---
decision:
  id: study_sample_size
  type: study_sample_size

  value: 400

  label: "We aim to recruit around 400 participants."

  justification:
    -
      id: aipe_for_correlations
      label: "To estimate a correlation accurately, you need ~ 400 participants."
      assertion:
        -
          id: nice_round_number
          label: "We want to recruit a nice round number of participants."
          source:
            id: nice_round_numbers_are_nice
            year: 219
            label: "In our meeting of 2019-07-05, we agreed that all team members like nice round numbers."
            evidence_type: "team opinion"
        -
          id: exact_aipe_for_our_study
          label: "Table 1 shows that 383 participants are sufficient even for correlations as low as .05."
          source:
            id: moinester_gottfried_2014
            year: 2014
            label: "Moinester, M., & Gottfried, R. (2014). Sample size estimation for correlations with pre-specified confidence interval. The Quantitative Methods of Psychology, 10(2), 124–130."
            evidence_type: "theory"
---

Pregistrations

---
decision:
  id: preregistration
  type: preregistration

  value: false

  label: "We will not preregister this study."

  justification:
    -
      id: low_risk_of_bias
      label: "Because we do not test a specific hypothesis but simply want to know how relevant the different determinants are in this population, the need to preregister is less pressing."
      assertion:
        -
          id: prereg_to_decrease_bias
          label: "Preregistration_decreases_bias"
          source:
            id: example_source
            label: "Not online now, but a study making this point would be nice here"
            evidence_type: "empirical study"
    -
      id: no_time_for_prereg
      label: "We have no time to complete a preregistration form."
      assertion:
        id: project_deadline
---

justifier processing

This vignette itself is a valid .jmd file (see here for the vignette Rmd file and here for the example .jmd file; the content is identical). It can be read by justifier, as is, to structure the information and apply the framework. If this framework specifies, for example (which this example framework happens to specify), that there exist different evidence types (a new field for source elements, as specified in the framework), and that each can be awarded a score based on some idea of evidential quality, it becomes possible to compute the mean score per decision. Because this vignette is contained within the package in the extdata directory as well, it can be read and processed here. Since justifier frameworks can also contain specifications of ways to verify the entered data, it will immediately spot the typo we made:

examplePath <- file.path(system.file(package="justifier"), 'extdata');

res <-
  justifier::load_justifications(file=file.path(examplePath,
                                                "study-example.jmd"),
                                 silent=TRUE);
#> 
#> 
#> Not all specifications of decisions, justifications, assertions, and sources successfully passed all verifications for framework '':
#> 
#> - In the source with identifier `nice_round_numbers_are_nice`, when applying a verification in framework condition `source_year`, it failed with message: "The year you specify in a source must consist of exactly four digits.".
#> 
#> 
#> All specifications of decisions, justifications, assertions, and sources successfully passed all verifications for framework ''.
#> 
#> All specifications of decisions, justifications, assertions, and sources successfully passed all verifications for framework ''.

Having successfully read the justifications, we can now show the mean score for every decision:

knitr::kable(res$fwApplications$`justifier-example-study-framework-specification`$scoreDf);

|| || || ||

We can also show overviews of each decision and its justifications:

plot(res$decisionGraphs$research_question);
plot(res$decisionGraphs$study_sample_size);

  1. Note that this framework is intended as an example, not as an authoritative framework that should be relied on in the wild.↩︎

  2. Again, this is a gross oversimplification; these are not the most important decisions, but they have been chosen because they cover different kinds of decisions, with different kinds of evidence available.↩︎

These binaries (installable software) and packages are in development.
They may not be fully stable and should be used with caution. We make no claims about them.
Health stats visible at Monitor.